Addicted to Brain Scans
The debate about sex addiction reflects a larger cultural confusion.

Last July, researchers at UCLA reported that the brains of people diagnosed as "hypersexual" do not display distinctive responses to sexual images. The study's authors interpreted their findings as evidence that hypersexuality, defined as a harmful and distressing preoccupation with sex, is not a true addiction or mental disorder.
A week after the UCLA study was published, Ariel Castro pleaded guilty to 937 criminal charges stemming from his kidnapping of three women whom he imprisoned and repeatedly raped over the course of a decade. "I'm not a monster," he insisted at his sentencing hearing. "I'm just sick. I have an addiction, just like an alcoholic has an addiction."
Around the same time, disgraced former congressman Anthony Weiner, who resigned in 2011 after admitting he had "sexted" photos of his genitals to various women, was attempting an improbable political comeback by seeking the Democratic nomination for mayor of New York City. His campaign suffered a major setback when it emerged that Weiner, a married man, had continued conducting virtual liaisons with Internet acquaintances even after quitting Congress.
All of these stories raise the question of how we should view people who don't control their sexual urges. Are they addicted to sex in the same way that an alcoholic is bonded to booze or a junkie is hooked on heroin? If so, does that mean they are no longer responsible for their behavior? And to what extent does neuroscientific evidence such as the electroencephalograms (EEGs) used in the UCLA study illuminate these issues? The debate about the nature of hypersexuality exemplifies a broader cultural confusion about addiction, a concept that has been medicalized to the point that people think it means both less and more than it really does.
If I Only Had a Brain Scan
The UCLA study, reported in the journal Socioaffective Neuroscience and Psychology, involved 52 volunteers whose scores on tests of sexual compulsivity were similar to those of "people seeking help for hypersexual problems." The researchers, led by the neuroscientist Nicole Prause, hooked the subjects up to electroencephalographs and showed them pictures that ranged from romantic to sexually explicit. Prause and her colleagues found that the brain responses registered by the machines were related to the subjects' self-reported levels of sexual arousal but not to their hypersexuality scores. "Hypersexuality does not appear to explain brain responses to sexual images any more than just having a high libido," Prause said in a university press release.
The researchers presented their results as evidence that hypersexuality is not really an addiction. "A diagnosis of hypersexuality or sexual addiction is typically associated with people who have sexual urges that feel out of control, who engage frequently in sexual behavior, who have suffered consequences such as divorce or economic ruin as a result of their behaviors, and who have a poor ability to reduce those behaviors," the press release noted. But according to Prause and her colleagues, "such symptoms are not necessarily representative of an addiction-in fact, nonpathological, high sexual desire could also explain this cluster of problems."
For someone unblinded by neuroscience, it is hard to make sense of this explanation. Sex addiction is defined by a pattern of behavior, a pattern exhibited by the subjects in this study, who reported that their sexual preoccupations and actions were so unrestrained that these had gotten them into trouble at home or work. Are these problems less real because their EEG tests were not abnormal? If the essence of addiction is a life-disrupting attachment that continues despite undesirable consequences, why does it matter whether Prause's subjects displayed a special "P300 response"?
Prause's reliance on EEG data to cast doubt on the reality of the problems reported by her subjects is an example of what I call "neuroreductionism": the tendency to perceive human experiences as valid or genuine only when they can be linked to measurable brain activity. In their recent book Brainwashed: The Seductive Appeal of Mindless Neuroscience, Oasis Clinic psychiatrist Sally Satel and Emory University psychologist Scott Lilienfeld detail the faulty science and logic underlying this mindset.
Neuroreductionism is the latest twist on the medicalization of addiction. Medicine has come up with wave after wave of biological explanations for addiction, ranging from the belief that narcotics such as heroin have special chemical effects that cause addiction to wild claims implicating the endorphins as addictive culprits. All these theories posit that addiction is caused by the things to which people become addicted.
My view, by contrast, is that people can become addicted to anything, whether drugs, alcohol, food, shopping, gambling, love, or sex, if it is the focus of an encapsulating experience that alleviates bad feelings and buttresses their self-esteem. Contrary to the common view of addiction as a choice-nullifying disease, this approach holds people accountable for their actions. Addicts are actively involved in building their attachments and can modify their behavior when they have an incentive to do so. Alcoholics drink moderately at home with their parents, for instance, and addicted smokers wait all morning during work until they can smoke outdoors. They might prefer to indulge their addictive impulses instantly, but those impulses can be resisted and ultimately eliminated.
Likewise, many highly sexual people do not act on their urges in ways that create problems. Even among those who qualify for the "hypersexual" label, the vast majority do not kidnap women and rape them. Since sex addicts clearly can conform to social expectations, legal limits, and moral principles, it is right to punish those who, like Ariel Castro, egregiously fail to do so.
Anthony Weiner's transgressions were trivial in comparison with Castro's. But his evident lack of self-control still prompted debate about exactly what his problem was.
"The candidate's behavior meets a fundamental criterion for addiction: his exhibitionist acts continued despite negative consequences," Time's health writer Maia Szalavitz claimed. "It's hard to imagine a better example of compulsive repetition: although he lost his job and put his marriage, family and entire political future at risk, the former Congressman nonetheless engaged again in the exact type of online behavior that brought him to public humiliation."
Yet Szalavitz was not ready to call Weiner an addict. "It's still not clear whether sexual compulsions qualify as an addiction," she said. "The [UCLA study] argued that they don't, because hypersexual people process sexual cues just like normal people do-and differently from the way addicts respond to drug cues. But the question is far from resolved. And whatever you want to call the issue, Weiner still has a problem, since compulsions can be just as disruptive as addictions, and equally difficult to overcome."
Szalavitz is at odds with herself. She says Weiner-who displayed the defining characteristic of addiction, persistence in destructive behavior-may not have been truly addicted because scientists found that sex addicts don't have special brain responses to sexual stimuli. Instead, Weiner may suffer from a destructive sexual compulsion. But that is a distinction without a difference. Szalavitz, who is as well-informed on addiction and drugs as anyone who has ever written for a mainstream news outlet, still gets tripped up by neuroreductionism. According to this way of thinking, if a person behaves like an addict but doesn't produce the right brain scan, he has to be called something else.
Before Cocaine Was Addictive
Although addiction historically referred to just about any strong attachment or hard-to-break habit, physicians began using the term to describe a medical syndrome at the turn of the 20th century. They had in mind addiction to drugs. Specifically, they had in mind addiction to opiates, since they erroneously believed that only opiates caused tolerance (a need for larger doses to achieve the same effect) and "physical dependence" (withdrawal symptoms). Alcohol and barbiturates were added to the list later, but physicians and pharmacologists continued to describe other drugs as merely "habituating." The U.S. surgeon general's landmark 1964 report on the health hazards of smoking, for instance, deemed nicotine habituating but not addictive. The same distinction was applied to cocaine, amphetamines, marijuana, and various other drugs.
By the late 1980s, the federal government had changed its mind: The 1988 surgeon general's report on smoking was titled Nicotine Addiction. The American Psychiatric Association likewise broadened its definition of addiction, noting in the fourth edition of its Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV, published in 1994) that "neither tolerance nor withdrawal is necessary or sufficient for a diagnosis of Substance Dependence." The DSM-IV defined substance dependence as "a maladaptive pattern of substance use" resulting in "clinically significant impairment or distress" meeting at least three out of seven criteria. It referred to less severe "substance use disorders" as "substance abuse."
The fifth edition of the manual-the DSM-5, published last May-eliminates the distinction between dependence and abuse. Instead it classifies substance use disorders as mild, moderate, or severe. Thus the DSM-5 does not explicitly recognize such a thing as drug addiction or dependence. But under "Substance Use and Addictive Disorders" the manual includes a category called "Behavioral Addictions" that so far consists of a lone entry: "Gambling Disorder." This term identifies what used to be called "pathological gambling," compulsive gambling that causes severe life problems.
It is unlikely that organized psychiatry will stop at labeling a single nondrug activity as addictive. Already the DSM-5 lists "Internet gaming disorder" as another possible behavioral addiction in Section III, which describes disorders under consideration for future inclusion in the manual. They're holding off for now because they want to see whether "persistent and recurrent use of Internet games, and a preoccupation with them, can result in clinically significant impairment or distress." But we already know that such preoccupations can disrupt people's lives, as commentator after commentator tells us they do, just as preoccupations with gambling, sex, and many other activities can. We are headed one way with addiction, which is to expand the concept.
I have advocated a broader understanding of addiction since 1975, when the Harvard psychiatry researcher Archie Brodsky and I published our book Love and Addiction. We argued not only that the distinction between "addictive" and "habituating" drugs should be abandoned but that nondrug activities, including love, sex, eating, and gambling, could also be addictive, or not, depending on the way people became involved in them and how destructive the involvement was. As we wrote in 1975, "If addiction is now known not to be primarily a matter of drug chemistry or body chemistry, and if we therefore have to broaden our conception of dependency-creating objects to include a wider range of drugs, then why stop with drugs? Why not look at the whole range of things, activities, and even people to which we can and do become addicted? We must, in fact, do this if addiction is to be made a viable concept once again."
While psychiatry is gradually moving in this direction, it is doing so under the pretense of locating addiction in the brain. In a February 2010 press release defending the decision to describe gambling as an "addictive disorder," the head of the DSM-5 working group on substance use disorders, Charles O'Brien, cited "substantive research" indicating that "pathological gambling and substance-use disorders are very similar in the way they affect the brain and neurological reward system." So according to O'Brien, gambling is addictive because it affects the brain as drugs do.
The physician and historian Howard Markel likewise defends the neuroscientific soundness of the DSM-5's approach. "The concept of addiction has been changing and expanding for centuries," he wrote in a 2012 New York Times essay. "Initially, it wasn't even a medical notion." (Imagine!) "'Addiction' entered the medical lexicon only in the late 19th century, as a result of the over-prescription of opium and morphine by physicians. Here, the concept of addiction came to include the notion of an exogenous substance taken into the body. Starting in the early 20th century, another key factor in diagnosing addiction was the occurrence of physical withdrawal symptoms upon quitting the substance in question."
Markel continues: "This definition of addiction was not always carefully applied (it took years for alcohol and nicotine to be classified as addictive, despite their fitting the bill), nor did it turn out to be accurate. Consider marijuana: in the 1980s, when I was training to become a doctor, marijuana was considered not to be addictive because the smoker rarely developed physical symptoms upon stopping. We now know that for some users marijuana can be terribly addictive, but because clearance of the drug from the body's fat cells takes weeks (instead of hours or days), physical withdrawal rarely occurs, though psychological withdrawal certainly can."
Instead of seeing addiction as a constantly shifting idea, Markel retrofits history to define addiction neurochemically. According to him, it was scientific progress when addiction was defined as use of a specific substance that might lead to physical withdrawal. Yet by his reckoning, it also was progress when those conditions were abandoned. Why? Brain scans!
Markel explains why he is now convinced that gambling, like marijuana, is addictive: "A team of scientists led by Nora Volkow at the National Institute on Drug Abuse have used positron emission tomography (PET) scans to show that even when cocaine addicts merely watch videos of people using cocaine, dopamine levels increase in the part of their brains associated with habit and learning. Dr. Volkow's group and other scientists have used PET scans and functional magnetic resonance imaging [fMRI] to demonstrate similar dopamine receptor derangements in the brains of drug addicts, compulsive gamblers and overeaters who are markedly obese." By this light, food is addictive and sex isn't.
Where Are the Sex Parts?
Section III of the DSM-5 does not include sex addiction, hypersexuality, or excessive pornography consumption, so evidently they are not being considered for later inclusion. That omission has prompted intense criticism from the UCLA psychologist Rory Reid. In a 2012 study of people treated for hypersexuality, Reid and his collaborators found that the proposed DSM-5 criteria for the condition were both reliable (leading to diagnostic agreement among clinicians) and valid (corresponding to real-world consequences). Conforming with an essential requirement for inclusion in the DSM, the criteria included "evidence of personal distress caused by the sexual behaviors that interfere with relationships, work or other important aspects of life."
The Reid study, which was reported in the Journal of Sexual Medicine, was produced at the same UCLA research center as the recent EEG study supposedly showing that hypersexuality is not a real addiction. "Science Supports Sex Addiction As a Legitimate Disorder," announced the UCLA press release heralding the 2012 study, while the headline on the press release about the 2013 study asked, "Is Sexual Addiction the Real Deal?"
The 2012 study involved no brain measurements, just DSM criteria. Yet no one is ever sent to rehab because of a brain scan. People are treated because of life problems and distress they have experienced. That's how the DSM-5 is written. Psychiatry's bible includes no defining criteria that refer to brain scans for the simple reason that there are no such defining criteria. Contrary to the claims of neuroscientific enthusiasts such as Charles O'Brien, Howard Markel, and Nora Volkow, you cannot see addiction on an fMRI.
The 2013 UCLA study rejected sex addiction because addicts did not show measurable brain reactions to sex like those cocaine addicts show in response to cocaine. But brain imaging studies of cocaine addiction do not find differences between experienced cocaine users who were addicted and those who were not. In other words, as in the case of sex addiction, a brain scan cannot indicate whether the subject is an addicted or controlled consumer, to what extent he might be addicted, or whether he is ready and able to cut back or quit.
A 2011 New York Times profile of Volkow, aptly headlined "A General in the Drug War," describes her scientific obsession this way: "She must say it a dozen times a day: Addiction is all about the dopamine." Dopamine is the neurochemical graphically imagined to mediate the pleasure centers of the brain, so that people become dependent on dopamine-producing experiences-which include, along with sex and eating, seeing a child smile, meditation, and being in love. But another obvious factor affects addictive behavior: self-restraint.
In a highly publicized 2012 study reported in the journal Science, Cambridge University researchers examined 47 addicted people along with 49 of their nonaddicted siblings. They found that the brains of both the addicted and nonaddicted siblings differed from those of 50 unrelated, nonaddicted people. Here is how Volkow described the results in an accompanying commentary: "The inferior frontal gyrus is really one of the main 'brakes' of our brain. [The drug users and their siblings] have less connections that are linking the rest of the brain with the inferior frontal gyrus that form a network that allows you to inhibit responses." But how does that explain why one sibling is addicted and the other isn't?
Covering the study in Time, Szalavitz made an observation that raises further questions: "Interestingly, the authors note, these connectivity problems are similar to those seen in the brains of teenagers, a group that is characterized by impulsive behavior. It is almost as if the brains of addicts are less mature. Perhaps that helps explain why some addiction wanes with age. Studies find that most people who struggle with alcohol and other drugs in their 20s [age out] of their problems by their 30s, typically without treatment." How is it that the overwhelming majority of people outgrow this brain disease, which the study suggests is inherited?
I'm an Addict, Not a Patient
There is a difference between saying a person with a life-disrupting attachment to something is addicted to it and saying he has a disease. In addition to arguing against unjustifiably restricted definitions of addiction, I have long resisted the excessive medicalization of human problems. This tendency can hurt the very people it is intended to help, as Szalavitz noted in a June 2013 Time essay.
Arguing that "defining obesity as a disease may do more harm than good," she drew an analogy to heavy drinking: "When alcoholism is seen as a disease, doctors and patients are also more likely to believe that overindulging can't be stopped without professional help or attending groups like AA-and that it must be treated with total abstinence. The disease concept wound up creating a ghettoized treatment system aimed only at severe cases, with few options for the vast majority of people with alcohol problems who don't require such extreme measures."
In addition to the question of whether it is helpful to call compulsive eating or drinking a disease, there is the question of whether it is accurate. Some critics of the DSM argue that the lack of biological tests for the disorders it lists undermines psychiatry's putative status as a branch of medicine. Thomas Insel, director of the National Institute of Mental Health, hopes further research will remedy the DSM's "lack of validity." As a May 2013 New York Times article explained, Insel wants to "reshape the direction of psychiatric research to focus on biology, genetics and neuroscience." But as the Times noted, "Decades of spending on neuroscience have taught scientists mostly what they do not know, undermining some of their most elemental assumptions."
When it comes to addiction, this is not a temporary inconvenience. It's the nature of the beast. Addiction can be defined only in terms of people's experiences and behaviors. There is nothing more scientific about how attached people become to an addictive object-whether heroin, alcohol, sex, eating, video games, or shopping-than to understand how subjectively captivating and powerful it is for them. That's not to say these things are usually or permanently addictive. Addiction is a specific involvement a person forms in a particular period in his life. Nothing more scientific can be said than that.
Editor's Note: As of February 29, 2024, commenting privileges on reason.com posts are limited to Reason Plus subscribers. Past commenters are grandfathered in for a temporary period. Subscribe here to preserve your ability to comment. Your Reason Plus subscription also gives you an ad-free version of reason.com, along with full access to the digital edition and archives of Reason magazine. We request that comments be civil and on-topic. We do not moderate or assume any responsibility for comments, which are owned by the readers who post them. Comments do not represent the views of reason.com or Reason Foundation. We reserve the right to delete any comment and ban commenters for any reason at any time. Comments may only be edited within 5 minutes of posting. Report abuses.
Please
to post comments
The whole sex addiction thing is an excuse pansy guys use for cheating on their significant other and wives use for cheating on their boyfriends or husbands.
"I couldn't help it! I'm addicted!"
No, your significant other probably isn't putting out enough, became intolerable, lacks confidence, and/or has let themselves go physically so you get some on the side.
Should've chosen wisely or never gotten married in the first place.
Warty: And how do we begin to covet, Shreek? Do we seek out things to covet? Make an effort to answer now.
Shreek: No. We just...
Warty: No. We begin by coveting what we see every day. Don't you feel eyes moving over your body, Shreek? And don't your eyes seek out the things you want?
So instead of blaming adddiction, cheaters should blame their significant other?
And themselves!
I concur, sex is not an addiction nor should it be treated as an illness, people just need to find those who are sexually accommodating to themselves instead of trying to force failed relationships with bad sex
Once again, South Park has it about right: addicts just rack-a-disciprine!
people who willfully engage in bad habits with consistent bad outcomes lack discipline, Addicts have to feed the addiction or suffer the consequences their own body will wreak upon them as it screams from every nerve ending for another fix
You clearly have no concept of this
After reading your article I was amazed. I know that you explain it very well. And I hope that other readers will also experience how I feel after reading your article. I feel very grateful that I read this. It is very helpful and very informative and I really learned a lot from it.
https://myspace.com/bubblegum_casting
I thought it was going to be some boring old post, but it really compensated for my time. I will post a link to this page on my blog. I am sure my visitors will locate that extremely usefulI thought it was going to be some boring old post, but it really compensated for my time. I will post a link to this page on my blog. I am sure my visitors will locate that extremely useful.
bubblegum casting
"For someone unblinded by neuroscience, it is hard to make sense of this explanation."
I've been blinded by neuro-science (and sex-addicted) since '82. miss sakamoto = way hot.
Bullshit, It took a week to get over physical withdrawl from alcohol and damn near a year of abstinence to be able to walk into a bar without sweating and getting anxious. this is addiction
on the converse
I play video games as much as humanly possible, however it has never once stopped me from keeping my house clean, taking care of my kid, eating food, or sleeping. without games my life continues without a trip to the ER from withdrawl in fact I haven't had more than 20 mins for it in 3 days
Bad habits are not addictions.. if there is no physical pain from quitting you haven't been truly addicted you just lack self control
this article does a huge dis-service to anyone who actually is having a problem, you cannot "out grow" addiction and you cant cure it, you either are predisposed or not and if you want to stop and find you cant go get help, AA is not a cult it works great because only other addicts will ever understand addiction, no science can define the feelings and the fear its something you have to live through to be able to talk about. fucking idiot
no science can define the feelings and the fear its something you have to live through to be able to talk about. fucking idiot
Yet just a few sentences previously you were willing to completely dismiss the feelings and fear that people with different bad habits have because they don't feel physical pain as a result of quitting. You can't have it both ways. If addiction is defined strictly in terms of physical withdrawal then the feelings and fears you're experiencing are as meaningless as the feelings and fears that anybody feels after kicking their habits that produce no physical discomfort. That was kind of the point of the article.
I never dismissed it, i just said its different, the point of the article was that the results were the same.
What I said is Addiction and Bad Habits are vastly different
you cannot send someone to rehab for 28 days and expect them to quit a bad habit on the inverse you can send someone to rehab for 28 days and expect that the physical withdrawal (addiction) has subsided from opiates, alcohol, or amphetamines
Addiction is the physical experience of pain from not having something
A bad habit is drinking again after a 28 day program, which may be a result of previous addiction but is not the result of current addiction, it is the result of personal choice at that point, granted many people fresh out of rehab cannot think very clearly and choose to start their addiction back up without seeking outside help.
Im not sure that it is helpful to attempt to widen the descriptive umbrella of the "addiction" label on the one hand and to attempt to combat the "medicalization" of addictive behavior on the other hand. Lets be frank: there *are* significant differences in what happens to someone who is addicted to heroin and someone who is "addicted" to food, or sex. The compulsive use of opiates is directly life threatening, withdrawal symptoms are incapacitating and in a caring environment necessitate pharmaceutical intervention (in the case of methadone, "cold-turkey" withdrawal symptoms alone can be life threatening). Heroin addicts and food/sex "addicts" should not be treated using the same methodology - abstinence, while not the only option, is a viable option for heroin addicts in a way that is simply not possible for food or drug addicts.
IMO, food or sex "addicts" do not require medical intervention of any kind. They require ethical and/or behavioral modification intervention. Its important to recognize that regardless of whatever spin psychologists might try to put on things, ethical and behavioral modification is not medicine. Pharmaceutical/medical-only intervention is sensical among heroin addicts in ways that it is not sensical for food/sex addicts. Things like methadone, naltrexone and suboxone should be available cheaply and easily for whoever needs them with proper education. Needle exchange and paraphernalia handouts allow heroin addicts to survive with significantly less risk of immediate death when looking solely at primary risks such as thrombosis and necrosis from using sh*t works(Im not talking about AIDS - giving sex addicts condoms is smart but is an AIDS intervention not an addiction intervention).
The "addiction" label has over 100 years of baggage, legal and social, associated with it. The idea that simply *being* an addict is grounds for immediate incarceration, even without evidence such as possession, is a widespread reality and an incredibly popular one with no serious opposition through state legislation like the Baker and Marchmann Acts (even so-called "legalization" people support "medical" intervention, a "medicine" by which of course is meant involuntary incarceration in a facility where you are forced to espouse a belief and god and admit your sins, etc.) For this reason alone, it is sensible to begin to *retract* the use of the "addiction" label and the creation of an alternate term for those who struggle with compulsive issues that do not involve substance use. I can't stress the distinction enough. If your problem is you f*ck too much, the solution is not to f*ck. You might want to f*ck, which would be the entire set of "symptoms" of addiction. I've known patients who have used multiple opiates including methadone who, if they were to simply stop, would have an aneurysm.
It's not the same.
Using "addiction" to refer exclusively to the psychological aspects of the compulsion may be more useful than carving out "real" addictions that require medical intervention from "non-addictions" that do not require medical intervention, even though the underlying compulsion - the behavioral problem - is the actual driver of both types. You don't get physically dependent on drugs to the extent you describe from the occasional hit - that's every bit as much a compulsion/behavioral problem as overeating or "wanting to fuck a lot" or any other pleasure-seeking activity. Having separate terms for the psychological problem and the physical withdrawal symptoms may be useful, but I'm not sure if throwing out a useful word because of historically ambiguous usage is necessarily the best way to accomplish it.
Not even close to the same, I still remember only bits and pieces coming out of alcoholism, I wouldn't have gone to rehab if I hadn't been forced to. At the time I was completely fine with being wasted off my ass 24/7 because i only considered it a bad habit since some person like the one who wrote this article wrote a similar one comparing things the opposite way, and since as an addict i will only recognize information that supports the idea that i am not an addict i took it as a license to drink myself into oblivion after rehab for another 3 years until I was losing my mind trying to stop but not being able to make it past 10AM without some kind of drink....sex, gambling, porn not so much
Came here for a lesson in love, but it turns out I suffer from a very sexy learning disability...
First of all, addicts in a recovery program are not taught to believe that they aren't responible for their actions - quite the contrary. This author continually makes comments about 12 step programs that are simply not true.
As for whether addiction is a disease, I find little point in arguing about it. The problem with all of the logic is this: any rational person wouldn't make those kinds of choices. But that's just it - you're making an assumption that an addicted person has full capacities of rational thinking. They don't, otherwise they wouldn't do the things they do.
What we all need to recognize is that we're dealing with people who's addiction has has changed how they think. The brain has created new pathways, and they are the wrong ones. So, however you describe it, it doesn't change the fact that breaking that behavior will take some work to create new pathways in the brain - very few people can do this over night and I've watched a lot of friends die because they thought they could do it without help. Sexual addictions are no different and we should'nt ignore the fact that people suffering from addictions often commit suicide.
Bottom line, you can consider addiction whatever you want - it doesn't change the realities. If you haven't experienced it first or second hand, then you probably aren't qualified to make many judgments on the issue. The reality is that people are harmed and that these behaviors almost always require professional help to fully recover.
thanks for these info. visit our web on Outbound Training Semarang.
please comment to improvement.
success for you all.
PUSAT PELATIHAN SEMARANG
former congressman Anthony Weiner, who resigned in 2011